Разделы презентаций


Lecture 21 SIMPLE SENTENCE

Содержание

PLAN1. Constituent structure notional parts of the sentence expanded and unexpanded sentences

Слайды и текст этой презентации

Слайд 1Lecture 21 SIMPLE SENTENCE

Lecture 21 SIMPLE SENTENCE

Слайд 2PLAN
1. Constituent structure
notional parts of the sentence
expanded

and unexpanded sentences

PLAN1. Constituent structure notional parts of the sentence expanded and unexpanded sentences

Слайд 3 complete and incomplete (elliptical) sentences
semantic classification of simple

sentences

complete and incomplete (elliptical) sentences semantic classification of simple sentences

Слайд 42. Paradigmatic structure
derivational procedures
clausalization and phrasalization
predicative functions

2. Paradigmatic structure derivational procedures clausalization and phrasalization predicative functions

Слайд 5Constituent structure.

Constituent structure.

Слайд 6the finite verb + the subject = the basic predicative

meaning of the sentence
= predicative line of the sentence

the finite verb + the subject = the basic predicative meaning of the sentence = predicative line

Слайд 7sentences are divided into:
monopredicative - one predicative line, i.e.

simple,
polypredicative = two or more predicative lines, i.e. composite and

semi-composite.
sentences are divided into: monopredicative - one predicative line, i.e. simple,polypredicative = two or more predicative lines,

Слайд 8a) notional parts of the sentence

a) notional parts of the sentence

Слайд 9 simple sentence = a system of function-expressing positions reflecting

certain element of situation.
Each position = the member

of the sentence.
They are arranged in a hierarchy  each of them modifying the others.
simple sentence = a system of function-expressing positions reflecting certain element of situation. Each position =

Слайд 10Hierarchy of members:

1) principal (main):
the subject
the predicate,

modify each other

Hierarchy of members: 1) principal (main):the subject the predicate,  modify each other

Слайд 11the subject is the “person” modifier of the predicate,
the

predicate is the “process” modifier of the subject;
they are

interdependent.
the subject is the “person” modifier of the predicate, the predicate is the “process” modifier of the

Слайд 122) secondary:
the object – a substance modifier of the predicate;


the attribute – a quality modifier of substantive parts, either

the subject or the object;
the apposition – a special kind of an attribute, a substance modifier of the subject;
2) secondary:the object – a substance modifier of the predicate; the attribute – a quality modifier of

Слайд 13the adverbial modifier – a quality modifier of the predicate;


the parenthesis (parenthetical enclosure) - a detached speaker-bound modifier either

of one of the nominative parts of the sentence or of the sentence in general: To be sure, Morris had treaded her badly. He probably won’t be able to make it today.

the adverbial modifier – a quality modifier of the predicate; the parenthesis (parenthetical enclosure) - a detached

Слайд 14the address (addressing enclosure) – a modifier of the destination

of the whole sentence;
the interjection (interjectional enclosure) – an

emotional modifier.
the address (addressing enclosure) – a modifier of the destination of the whole sentence; the interjection (interjectional

Слайд 15 nominative parts of the sentence are syntagmatically connected,


the relations between them can be representned in

a linear as well as in a hierarchical way
nominative parts of the sentence are syntagmatically connected,   the relations between them can

Слайд 16linear analisys
My child always obeys me.

linear analisysMy child always obeys me.

Слайд 17IC analisys
My child always obeys me.
Pron
N
D
V


Pron
NP
VP
VP
S

IC analisysMy child always obeys me. Pron N D V Pron NP VP VP S

Слайд 18b) expanded and unexpanded sentences

b) expanded and unexpanded sentences

Слайд 19may or may not be actually represented in the sentence.


This is determined by the valency of the verb-predicate
Sentence parts
obligatory
optional

may or may not be actually represented in the sentence. This is determined by the valency of

Слайд 20the category of “elementary sentence”
= a sentence in

which all the positions are obligatory
(the principal parts +

complementive modifiers).
the category of “elementary sentence” = a sentence in which all the positions are obligatory (the principal

Слайд 21 Simple sentences can be:
unexpanded = elementary sentence,

includes only obligatory nominative parts;
expanded - includes some optional parts,

i.e. supplementive modifiers, which do not change the simple sentence into a composite or semi-composite sentence.
Simple sentences can be:unexpanded = elementary sentence, includes only obligatory nominative parts;expanded - includes

Слайд 22‘He gave me the book’
unexpanded - all the nominative

parts of this sentence are required by the obligatory valency

of the verb to give;
cf.: *He gave…; He gave me… - semantically and structurally deficient.
‘He gave me the book’ unexpanded - all the nominative parts of this sentence are required by

Слайд 23‘He gave me a very interesting book’
expanded - includes

the attribute-supplement very interesting;
is reducible to the elementary unexpanded

sentence
‘He gave me a very interesting book’ expanded - includes the attribute-supplement very interesting; is reducible to

Слайд 24c) complete and incomplete (elliptical) sentences

c) complete and incomplete (elliptical) sentences

Слайд 25 the axes of the sentence:
the subject group

(the subject axis)
the predicate group (the predicate axis).
the

subject and the predicate

the subordinate secondary parts

+

 the axes of the sentence: the subject group (the subject axis) the predicate group (the predicate

Слайд 26Sentence with both axes present  complete sentences (“two-member sentences”

or “two-axis sentences”).
Sentence with one of the axes present

 incomplete sentences, (“one-member sentence”, “one-axis sentence) e.g.: What a nice day!
Sentence with both axes present  complete sentences (“two-member sentences” or “two-axis sentences”). Sentence with one of

Слайд 27free one-axis sent. – the zero axis can be easily

restored from the conext. Elliptical sentences: e.g.: Who is there?

– Your brother.
fixed one-axis sent. - the absent axis cannot be easily and accurately restored.
free one-axis sent. – the zero axis can be easily restored from the conext. Elliptical sentences: e.g.:

Слайд 28Examples of fixed one-axis sentences:
emotionally colored name-callings,

e.g.: Brute!;
psychologically tense descriptions,

e.g.: Night. Silence. No one in sight (so-called nominative sentences);
Examples of fixed one-axis sentences: emotionally colored name-callings,     e.g.: Brute!; psychologically tense descriptions,

Слайд 29various emphatic constructions,

e.g.: To ask a question

like this! What a joy!;
some conversational formulas, e.g.: Thank you! Nice meeting you!; etc.
various emphatic constructions,             e.g.: To

Слайд 30BUT!
negation and affirmation formulas (Yes; No; All right),
vocative sentences

(Ladies and gentlemen! Dear friends!),
greeting and parting formulas (Hello!

Good-bye!)
 belong to the periphery of the category of the sentence
BUT!negation and affirmation formulas (Yes; No; All right), vocative sentences (Ladies and gentlemen! Dear friends!), greeting and

Слайд 31 + exclamations of interjectional type, like My God!

For heaven’s sake! Gosh!, etc.,
= “pseudo-sentences”, or

“non-communicative utterances”
 render no situational nomination, predication or informative perspective of any kind
+ exclamations of interjectional type, like My God! For heaven’s sake! Gosh!, etc.,

Слайд 32d) semantic classification of simple sentences

d) semantic classification of simple sentences

Слайд 33The semantic classification of simple sentences is based on principal

parts semantics.

The semantic classification of simple sentences is based on principal parts semantics.

Слайд 34A. On the basis of subject categorial meaning, sentences are

divided into
1) impersonal, e.g.: It drizzles; There is no use

crying over spilt milk;
a) factual, e.g.: It drizzles;
b) perceptional, e.g. It looks like rain. It smells of hay here.

A. On the basis of subject categorial meaning, sentences are divided into1) impersonal, e.g.: It drizzles; There

Слайд 352) personal;
a) human
definite, e.g.: I know it;
indefinite, e.g.:

One never knows such things for sure.

2) personal; a) human definite, e.g.: I know it;indefinite, e.g.: One never knows such things for sure.

Слайд 36b) non-human.
animate, e.g.: A cat entered the room;
inanimate,

e.g.: The wind opened the door.

b) non-human. animate, e.g.: A cat entered the room; inanimate, e.g.: The wind opened the door.

Слайд 37B. On the basis of predicate categorial meaning, sentences are

divided into
1) process featuring (“verbal”)
a) actional, e.g.: I play

ball;
b) statal, e.g.: I enjoy your party;
B. On the basis of predicate categorial meaning, sentences are divided into1) process featuring (“verbal”) a) actional,

Слайд 382) substance featuring (“nominal”);
a) factual, e.g.: She is clever;


b) perceptional, e.g.: She seems to be clever.

2) substance featuring (“nominal”); a) factual, e.g.: She is clever; b) perceptional, e.g.: She seems to be

Слайд 39C. subdivisions of simple sentences based on the structure of

the predicate:
predicates:
simple (I read)
compound,

C. subdivisions of simple sentences based on the structure of the predicate: predicates: simple (I read) compound,

Слайд 40 compound,
compound nominal predicates with pure and specifying link

verbs (She looked beautiful).

compound, compound nominal predicates with pure and specifying link verbs (She looked beautiful).

Слайд 41compound verbal predicates
CV modal Pr (You can prove it)
CV

aspect Pr (She started crying)
Mixed types

compound verbal predicates CV modal Pr (You can prove it)CV aspect Pr (She started crying) Mixed types

Слайд 42D. On the basis of subject-object relations, simple sentences are

divided into
subjective, e.g.: He is a writer;
objective, e.g.:

He is writing a book;
neutral or potentially objective, e.g.: He is writing.
D. On the basis of subject-object relations, simple sentences are divided into subjective, e.g.: He is a

Слайд 432. Paradigmatic structure.

2. Paradigmatic structure.

Слайд 44 Traditionally, the sentence was studied only syntagmatically.
F. de

Saussure: paradigmatics is quite natural for morphology, while syntax should

be studied primarily as the linear connections of words.
Traditionally, the sentence was studied only syntagmatically. F. de Saussure: paradigmatics is quite natural for morphology,

Слайд 45Regular paradigmatic description of syntax started in the middle of

the 20th century
(N.Chomsky’s transformational grammar theory).

Regular paradigmatic description of syntax started in the middle of the 20th century (N.Chomsky’s transformational grammar theory).

Слайд 46various sentence patterns
 various functional meanings
They make up

syntactic categories = the oppositions of paradigmatically correlated sentence patterns.


various sentence patterns  various functional meanings They make up syntactic categories = the oppositions of paradigmatically

Слайд 47Study of these oppositions  distinguish formal marks and individual

grammatical meanings of paradigmatically opposed sentence patterns.

Study of these oppositions  distinguish formal marks and individual grammatical meanings of paradigmatically opposed sentence patterns.

Слайд 48a) derivational procedures

a) derivational procedures

Слайд 49syntactic derivation starts with
the kernel sentence
= the elementary

sentence (the principal parts + complementive modifiers)
e.g.: Mary put

the book on the table.
syntactic derivation starts with the kernel sentence = the elementary sentence (the principal parts + complementive modifiers)

Слайд 50Derivation of a sentence
= several transformational steps


Derivation of a sentence = several transformational steps

Слайд 51transformational steps
morphological arrangement of the sentence parts (word

forms within categories)
- changes of the finite form of

the verb
e.g.: Mary put the book on the table  Mary would have put the book on the table.


transformational steps morphological arrangement of the sentence parts (word forms within categories) - changes of the finite

Слайд 52 the use of functional words (functional expansion), which transform

syntactic constructions
e.g.: Mary put the book on the table. 

Did Mary put the book on the table?
He understood my question.  He seemed to understand my question.
the use of functional words (functional expansion), which transform syntactic constructionse.g.: Mary put the book on

Слайд 53 the process of substitution, (the use of personal, demonstrative

and indefinite pronouns and of various substitutive half-notional words),
e.g.:

Mary put the book on the table.  Mary put it on the table.
I want another pen, please.  I want another one, please.
the process of substitution, (the use of personal, demonstrative and indefinite pronouns and of various substitutive

Слайд 544) deletion, i.e. elimination of some elements in various contextual

conditions,
e.g.: Put the book on the table!  On

the table!
4) deletion, i.e. elimination of some elements in various contextual conditions, e.g.: Put the book on the

Слайд 55 the process of positional arrangement, (changes of the word

order)
e.g.: Mary put the book on the table. 

On the table Mary put the book.
We must go.  Must we go?
the process of positional arrangement, (changes of the word order) e.g.: Mary put the book on

Слайд 566) the process of intonational arrangement, i.e. application of various

functional tones and accents,
e.g.: Mary put the book on

the table.  Mary put the book on the table?(!)
6) the process of intonational arrangement, i.e. application of various functional tones and accents, e.g.: Mary put

Слайд 57These steps may be employed either alone or in combination

with each other;
e.g. Where did Mary put the book?


the kernel sentence Mary put the book on the table,

These steps may be employed either alone or in combination with each other; e.g. Where did Mary

Слайд 58Types of derivational relations in the paradigmatic system of sentences:


constructional relations - the formation of more complex syntactic structures

out of simpler ones,
predicative relations - expression of the predicative semantics of the sentence.
Types of derivational relations in the paradigmatic system of sentences: constructional relations - the formation of more

Слайд 59b) clausalization and phrasalization

b) clausalization and phrasalization

Слайд 60
kernel sentences
transforms
clauses
phrases

kernel sentencestransformsclausesphrases

Слайд 61 clausalization = the transformation of a base sentence into

a clause in the process of the subordinative or coordinative

combination of sentences.

clausalization = the transformation of a base sentence into a clause in the process of the

Слайд 62 use of conjunctive words;
the change of the word

order;
the change of intonational arrangement, deletion, substitution and other

derivational procedures may be involved.
use of conjunctive words; the change of the word order; the change of intonational arrangement, deletion,

Слайд 63Cf.: The team won.
+ It caused a sensation.
 The

team won and it caused a sensation; When the team

won, it caused a sensation.
Cf.: The team won. + It caused a sensation. The team won and it caused a sensation;

Слайд 64phrasalization = the transformation of a base sentence into a

phrase in the process of building the syntactic constructions of

various degrees of complexity.
phrasalization = the transformation of a base sentence into a phrase in the process of building the

Слайд 65types of phrasalization:
nominalization, i.e. the transformation of a sentence

into a nominal phrase;

types of phrasalization: nominalization, i.e. the transformation of a sentence into a nominal phrase;

Слайд 66►complete nominalization the kernel sentence

 a regular noun phrase
 NO predicative semantics,

e.g.: The team won.  the team’s victory; The weather changed.  the change of the weather;
►complete nominalization      the kernel sentence  a regular noun phrase  NO

Слайд 67► partial nominalization

the sentence  a semi-predicative gerundial or infinitive

phrase
 part of its predicative semantics is lost,
e.g.: the team’s winning; for the team to win; the weather changing.
► partial nominalization          the sentence  a semi-predicative

Слайд 68c) predicative functions

c) predicative functions

Слайд 69 a kernel sentence undergoes transformations connected with the expression

of predicative syntactic semantics

a kernel sentence undergoes transformations connected with the expression of predicative syntactic semantics

Слайд 70Predicative functions, expressed by primary sentence patterns, can be subdivided

into
1. lower - include the expression of such morphological

categories as tense and aspect; they have “factual”, “truth-stating” semantic character.
Predicative functions, expressed by primary sentence patterns, can be subdivided into 1. lower - include the expression

Слайд 712. higher, “evaluative”; they are expressed by syntactic categorial oppositions,


they make up the following syntactic categories:

2. higher, “evaluative”; they are expressed by syntactic categorial oppositions,  they make up the following syntactic

Слайд 721) the category of communicative purpose:
the first sub-category -

question is opposed to statement,

cf..: Mary put the book on the table. – Did Mary put the book on the table?;
the second sub-category - statement is opposed to inducement, e.g.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary, put the book on the table;
1) the category of communicative purpose: the first sub-category - question is opposed to statement,

Слайд 73the category of existence quality (affirmation and negation) - affirmation

is opposed to negation, cf.: Mary put the book on

the table. – Mary didn’t put the book on the table;
the category of realization - unreality is opposed to reality, cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary would have put the book on the table…;
the category of existence quality (affirmation and negation) - affirmation is opposed to negation, cf.: Mary put

Слайд 74the category of probability - probability is opposed to fact,

cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary

might put he book on the table;
the category of modal identity - modal identity is opposed to fact, cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary happened to put the book on the table;
the category of probability - probability is opposed to fact, cf.: Mary put the book on the

Слайд 75the category of subjective modality, - modal subject-action relation is

opposed to fact,

cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary must put the book on the table;
the category of subject-action relations, - specified actual subject-action relation is opposed to fact, cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary tried to put the book on the table;
the category of subjective modality, - modal subject-action relation is opposed to fact,

Слайд 76the category of phase - phase of action is opposed

to fact,

cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary started putting her book on the table (though I asked her not to);
the category of subject-object relations - passive action is opposed to active action, cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – The book was put on the table by Mary;
the category of phase - phase of action is opposed to fact,

Слайд 77the category of informative perspective - specialized, reverse actual division

is opposed to non-specialized, direct actual division,

cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – It was Mary who put the book on the table;
the category of (emotional) intensity - emphasis (emotiveness) is opposed to emotional neutrality, cf.: Mary put the book on the table. – Mary did put the book on the table!
the category of informative perspective - specialized, reverse actual division is opposed to non-specialized, direct actual division,

Слайд 78 The total volume of the strong members of predicative

oppositions actually represented in a sentence = its predicative load.


The total volume of the strong members of predicative oppositions actually represented in a sentence =

Слайд 79 The kernel sentence, which is characterized in oppositional terms

as non-interrogative, non-imperative, non-negative, non-modal-identifying, etc., = predicatively “non-loaded” (has

a “zero predicative load”);
The kernel sentence, which is characterized in oppositional terms as non-interrogative, non-imperative, non-negative, non-modal-identifying, etc., =

Слайд 80 sentences with the most typical predicative loads of one

or two positive feature expressed = lightly loaded;

sentences with the most typical predicative loads of one or two positive feature expressed = lightly

Слайд 81sentences with predicative semantics of more than two positive predicative

features (normally, no more than six) are heavily loaded.

sentences with predicative semantics of more than two positive predicative features (normally, no more than six) are

Слайд 82Why on earth has Mary failed to put my book

back on the table?!
 expressing positive predicative semantics of

interrogations, subject-action relations and intensity;
 its predicative load is heavy.
Why on earth has Mary failed to put my book back on the table?!  expressing positive

Обратная связь

Если не удалось найти и скачать доклад-презентацию, Вы можете заказать его на нашем сайте. Мы постараемся найти нужный Вам материал и отправим по электронной почте. Не стесняйтесь обращаться к нам, если у вас возникли вопросы или пожелания:

Email: Нажмите что бы посмотреть 

Что такое TheSlide.ru?

Это сайт презентации, докладов, проектов в PowerPoint. Здесь удобно  хранить и делиться своими презентациями с другими пользователями.


Для правообладателей

Яндекс.Метрика